목록으로

공개 정명고 모고 내용이해워크북 제작 완료
내용 이해 워크북
최*주
2024-11-08 23:55:02

제작된 시험지/답지 다운로드 (총 392문제)
전체 파일 한번에 다운로드 하기
개별 파일 다운로드 및 미리보기

설정
시험지 제작 소요 포인트: 98 포인트
한글 OX 문제 수 1포인트/5문제,1지문 0
영어 OX 문제 수 1포인트/5문제,1지문 10
영한 해석 적기 문제 수 1포인트/5문제,1지문 3
스크램블 문제 수 2포인트/5문제,1지문 3
단어 뜻 적기 문제 수 1포인트/10문제,1지문 10
내용 이해 질문 문제 수 1포인트/5문제,1지문 0
지문 요약 적기 문제 수 2포인트/5문제,1지문 2
반복 생성 시험지 세트 수 1
지문 (14개)
# 영어 지문 지문 출처
지문 1
When we see something, we naturally and automatically break it up into shapes, colors, and concepts that we have learned through education. We recode what we see through the lens of everything we know. We reconstruct memories rather than retrieving the video from memory. This is a useful trait. It's a more efficient way to store information ─ a bit like an optimal image compression algorithm such as JPG, rather than storing a raw bitmap image file. People who lack this ability and remember everything in perfect detail struggle to generalize, learn, and make connections between what they have learned. But representing the world as abstract ideas and features comes at a cost of seeing the world as it is. Instead, we see the world through our assumptions, motivations, and past experiences. The discovery that our memories are reconstructed through abstract representations rather than played back like a movie completely undermined the legal primacy of eyewitness testimony. Seeing is not believing.
지문 2
In his Cornell laboratory, David Dunning conducted experimental tests of eyewitness testimony and found evidence that a careful deliberation of facial features and a detailed discussion of selection procedures can actually be a sign of an inaccurate identification. It's when people find themselves unable to explain why they recognize the person, saying things like his face just popped out at me, that they tend to be accurate more often. Sometimes our first, immediate, automatic reaction to a situation is the truest interpretation of what our mind is telling us. That very first impression can also be more accurate about the world than the deliberative, reasoned self-narrative can be. In his book Blink, Malcolm Gladwell describes a variety of studies in psychology and behavioral economics that demonstrate the superior performance of relatively unconscious first guesses compared to logical step-by-step justifications for a decision.
지문 3
Many forms of research lead naturally to quantitative data. A study of happiness might measure the number of times someone smiles during an interaction, and a study of memory might measure the number of items an individual can recall after one, five, and ten minutes. Asking people how many times in a year they are sad will also yield quantitative data, but it might not be reliable. Respondents' recollections may be inaccurate, and their definitions of 'sad' could vary widely. But asking How many times in the past year were you sad enough to call in sick to work? prompts a concrete answer. Similarly, instead of asking people to rate how bad a procrastinator they are, ask, How many of your utility bills are you currently late in paying, even though you can afford to pay them? Questions that seek concrete responses help make abstract concepts clearer and ensure consistency from one study to the next.
지문 4
The evolution of AI is often associated with the concept of singularity. Singularity refers to the point at which AI exceeds human intelligence. After that point, it is predicted that AI will repeatedly improve itself and evolve at an accelerated pace. When AI becomes self-aware and pursues its own goals, it will be a conscious being, not just a machine. AI and human consciousness will then begin to evolve together. Our consciousness will evolve to new dimensions through our interactions with AI, which will provide us with intellectual stimulation and inspire new insights and creativity. Conversely, our consciousness also has a significant impact on the evolution of AI. The direction of AI's evolution will depend greatly on what values and ethics we incorporate into AI. We need to see our relationship with AI as a mutual coexistence of conscious beings, recognizing its rights and supporting the evolution of its consciousness.
지문 5
Digital technologies are essentially related to metaphors, but digital metaphors are different from linguistic ones in important ways. Linguistic metaphors are passive, in the sense that the audience needs to choose to actively enter the world proposed by metaphor. In the Shakespearean metaphor time is a beggar, the audience is unlikely to understand the metaphor without cognitive effort and without further engaging Shakespeare's prose. Technological metaphors, on the other hand, are active (and often imposing) in the sense that they are realized in digital artifacts that are actively doing things, forcefully changing a user's meaning horizon. Technological creators cannot generally afford to require their potential audience to wonder how the metaphor works; normally the selling point is that the usefulness of the technology is obvious at first glance. Shakespeare, on the other hand, is beloved in part because the meaning of his works is not immediately obvious and requires some thought on the part of the audience.
지문 6
Herbert Simon won his Nobel Prize for recognizing our limitations in information, time, and cognitive capacity. As we lack the resources to compute answers independently, we distribute the computation across the population and solve the answer slowly, generation by generation. Then all we have to do is socially learn the right answers. You don't need to understand how your computer or toilet works; you just need to be able to use the interface and flush. All that needs to be transmitted is which button to push ─ essentially how to interact with technologies rather than how they work. And so instead of holding more information than we have mental capacity for and indeed need to know, we could dedicate our large brains to a small piece of a giant calculation. We understand things well enough to benefit from them, but all the while we are making small calculations that contribute to a larger whole. We are just doing our part in a larger computation for our societies' collective brains.
지문 7
The best defence most species of octopus have is to stay hidden as much as possible and do their own hunting at night. So to find one in full view in the shallows in daylight was a surprise for two Australian underwater photographers. Actually, what they saw at first was a flounder. It was only when they looked again that they saw a medium-sized octopus, with all eight of its arms folded and its two eyes staring upwards to create the illusion. An octopus has a big brain, excellent eyesight and the ability to change colour and pattern, and this one was using these assets to turn itself into a completely different creature. Many more of this species have been found since then, and there are now photographs of octopuses that could be said to be transforming into sea snakes. And while they mimic, they hunt ─ producing the spectacle of, say, a flounder suddenly developing an octopodian arm, sticking it down a hole and grabbing whatever's hiding there.
지문 8
How much we suffer relates to how we frame the pain in our mind. When 1500m runners push themselves into extreme pain to win a race ─ their muscles screaming and their lungs exploding with oxygen deficit, they don't psychologically suffer much. In fact, ultramarathon runners ─ those people who are crazy enough to push themselves beyond the normal boundaries of human endurance, covering distances of 50-100km or more over many hours, talk about making friends with their pain. When a patient has paid for some form of passive back pain therapy and the practitioner pushes deeply into a painful part of a patient's back to mobilise it, the patient calls that good pain if he or she believes this type of deep pressure treatment will be of value, even though the practitioner is pushing right into the patient's sore tissues.
지문 9
When I worked for a large electronics company that manufactured laser and inkjet printers, I soon discovered why there are often three versions of many consumer goods. If the manufacturer makes only one version of its product, people who bought it might have been willing to spend more money, so the company is losing some income. If the company offers two versions, one with more features and more expensive than the other, people will compare the two models and still buy the less expensive one. But if the company introduces a third model with even more features and more expensive than the other two, sales of the second model go up; many people like the features of the most expensive model, but not the price. The middle item has more features than the least expensive one, and it is less expensive than the fanciest model. They buy the middle item, unaware that they have been manipulated by the presence of the higher-priced item.
지문 10
Onscreen, climate disaster is everywhere you look, but the scope of the world's climate transformation may just as quickly eliminate the climate-fiction genre ─ indeed eliminate any effort to tell the story of warming, which could grow too large and too obvious even for Hollywood. You can tell stories 'about' climate change while it still seems a marginal feature of human life. But when the temperature rises by three or four more degrees, hardly anyone will be able to feel isolated from its impacts. And so as climate change expands across the horizon, it may cease to be a story. Why watch or read climate fiction about the world you can see plainly out your own window? At the moment, stories illustrating global warming can still offer an escapist pleasure, even if that pleasure often comes in the form of horror. But when we can no longer pretend that climate suffering is distant ─ in time or in place ─ we will stop pretending about it and start pretending within it.
지문 11
Today, the water crisis is political ─ which is to say, not inevitable or beyond our capacity to fix ─ and, therefore, functionally elective. That is one reason it is nevertheless distressing: an abundant resource made scarce through governmental neglect and indifference, bad infrastructure and contamination, and careless urbanization. There is no need for a water crisis, in other words, but we have one anyway, and aren't doing much to address it. Some cities lose more water to leaks than they deliver to homes: even in the United States, leaks and theft account for an estimated loss of 16 percent of freshwater; in Brazil, the estimate is 40 percent. Seen in both cases, as everywhere, the selective scarcity clearly highlights have-and-have-not inequities, leaving 2.1 billion people without safe drinking water and 4.5 billion without proper sanitation worldwide.
지문 12
As individuals, our ability to thrive depended on how well we navigated relationships in a group. If the group valued us, we could count on support, resources, and probably a mate. If it didn't, we might get none of these merits. It was a matter of survival, physically and genetically. Over millions of years, the pressure selected for people who are sensitive to and skilled at maximizing their standing. The result was the development of a tendency to unconsciously monitor how other people in our community perceive us. We process that information in the form of self-esteem and such related emotions as pride, shame, or insecurity. These emotions compel us to do more of what makes our community value us and less of what doesn't. And, crucially, they are meant to make that motivation feel like it is coming from within. If we realized, on a conscious level, that we were responding to social pressure, our performance might come off as grudging or cynical, making it less persuasive.
지문 13
Conventional medicine has long believed that depression is caused by an imbalance of neurotransmitters in the brain. However, there is a major problem with this explanation. This is because the imbalance of substances in the brain is a consequence of depression, not its cause. In other words, depression causes a decrease in brain substances such as serotonin and noradrenaline, not a decrease in brain substances causes depression. In this revised cause-and-effect, the key is to reframe depression as a problem of consciousness. Our consciousness is a more fundamental entity that goes beyond the functioning of the brain. The brain is no more than an organ of consciousness. If it is not consciousness itself, then the root cause of depression is also a distortion of our state of consciousness: a consciousness that has lost its sense of self and the meaning of life. Such a disease of consciousness may manifest itself in the form of depression.
지문 14
The common accounts of human nature that float around in society are generally a mixture of assumptions, tales and sometimes plain silliness. However, psychology is different. It is the branch of science that is devoted to understanding people: how and why we act as we do; why we see things as we do; and how we interact with one another. The key word here is 'science.' Psychologists don't depend on opinions and hearsay, or the generally accepted views of society at the time, or even the considered opinions of deep thinkers. Instead, they look for evidence, to make sure that psychological ideas are firmly based, and not just derived from generally held beliefs or assumptions. In addition to this evidence-based approach, psychology deals with fundamental processes and principles that generate our rich cultural and social diversity, as well as those shared by all human beings. These are what modern psychology is all about.
✅: 출제 대상 문장, ❌: 출제 제외 문장
    해석 스크램블 문장
지문 1 1. When we see something, we naturally and automatically break it up into shapes, colors, and concepts that we have learned through education.
2. We recode what we see through the lens of everything we know.
3. We reconstruct memories rather than retrieving the video from memory.
4. This is a useful trait.
5. It's a more efficient way to store information - a bit like an optimal image compression algorithm such as JPG, rather than storing a raw bitmap image file.
6. People who lack this ability and remember everything in perfect detail struggle to generalize, learn, and make connections between what they have learned.
7. But representing the world as abstract ideas and features comes at a cost of seeing the world as it is.
8. Instead, we see the world through our assumptions, motivations, and past experiences.
9. The discovery that our memories are reconstructed through abstract representations rather than played back like a movie completely undermined the legal primacy of eyewitness testimony.
10. Seeing is not believing.
지문 2 1. In his Cornell laboratory, David Dunning conducted experimental tests of eyewitness testimony and found evidence that a careful deliberation of facial features and a detailed discussion of selection procedures can actually be a sign of an inaccurate identification.
2. It's when people find themselves unable to explain why they recognize the person, saying things like his face just popped out at me, that they tend to be accurate more often.
3. Sometimes our first, immediate, automatic reaction to a situation is the truest interpretation of what our mind is telling us.
4. That very first impression can also be more accurate about the world than the deliberative, reasoned self-narrative can be.
5. In his book Blink, Malcolm Gladwell describes a variety of studies in psychology and behavioral economics that demonstrate the superior performance of relatively unconscious first guesses compared to logical step-by-step justifications for a decision.
지문 3 1. Many forms of research lead naturally to quantitative data.
2. A study of happiness might measure the number of times someone smiles during an interaction, and a study of memory might measure the number of items an individual can recall after one, five, and ten minutes.
3. Asking people how many times in a year they are sad will also yield quantitative data, but it might not be reliable.
4. Respondents' recollections may be inaccurate, and their definitions of 'sad' could vary widely.
5. But asking How many times in the past year were you sad enough to call in sick to work?
6. prompts a concrete answer.
7. Similarly, instead of asking people to rate how bad a procrastinator they are, ask, How many of your utility bills are you currently late in paying, even though you can afford to pay them?
8. Questions that seek concrete responses help make abstract concepts clearer and ensure consistency from one study to the next.
지문 4 1. The evolution of AI is often associated with the concept of singularity.
2. Singularity refers to the point at which AI exceeds human intelligence.
3. After that point, it is predicted that AI will repeatedly improve itself and evolve at an accelerated pace.
4. When AI becomes self-aware and pursues its own goals, it will be a conscious being, not just a machine.
5. AI and human consciousness will then begin to evolve together.
6. Our consciousness will evolve to new dimensions through our interactions with AI, which will provide us with intellectual stimulation and inspire new insights and creativity.
7. Conversely, our consciousness also has a significant impact on the evolution of AI.
8. The direction of AI's evolution will depend greatly on what values and ethics we incorporate into AI.
9. We need to see our relationship with AI as a mutual coexistence of conscious beings, recognizing its rights and supporting the evolution of its consciousness.
지문 5 1. Digital technologies are essentially related to metaphors, but digital metaphors are different from linguistic ones in important ways.
2. Linguistic metaphors are passive, in the sense that the audience needs to choose to actively enter the world proposed by metaphor.
3. In the Shakespearean metaphor time is a beggar, the audience is unlikely to understand the metaphor without cognitive effort and without further engaging Shakespeare's prose.
4. Technological metaphors, on the other hand, are active (and often imposing) in the sense that they are realized in digital artifacts that are actively doing things, forcefully changing a user's meaning horizon.
5. Technological creators cannot generally afford to require their potential audience to wonder how the metaphor works; normally the selling point is that the usefulness of the technology is obvious at first glance.
6. Shakespeare, on the other hand, is beloved in part because the meaning of his works is not immediately obvious and requires some thought on the part of the audience.
지문 6 1. Herbert Simon won his Nobel Prize for recognizing our limitations in information, time, and cognitive capacity.
2. As we lack the resources to compute answers independently, we distribute the computation across the population and solve the answer slowly, generation by generation.
3. Then all we have to do is socially learn the right answers.
4. You don't need to understand how your computer or toilet works; you just need to be able to use the interface and flush.
5. All that needs to be transmitted is which button to push - essentially how to interact with technologies rather than how they work.
6. And so instead of holding more information than we have mental capacity for and indeed need to know, we could dedicate our large brains to a small piece of a giant calculation.
7. We understand things well enough to benefit from them, but all the while we are making small calculations that contribute to a larger whole.
8. We are just doing our part in a larger computation for our societies' collective brains.
지문 7 1. The best defence most species of octopus have is to stay hidden as much as possible and do their own hunting at night.
2. So to find one in full view in the shallows in daylight was a surprise for two Australian underwater photographers.
3. Actually, what they saw at first was a flounder.
4. It was only when they looked again that they saw a medium-sized octopus, with all eight of its arms folded and its two eyes staring upwards to create the illusion.
5. An octopus has a big brain, excellent eyesight and the ability to change colour and pattern, and this one was using these assets to turn itself into a completely different creature.
6. Many more of this species have been found since then, and there are now photographs of octopuses that could be said to be transforming into sea snakes.
7. And while they mimic, they hunt - producing the spectacle of, say, a flounder suddenly developing an octopodian arm, sticking it down a hole and grabbing whatever's hiding there.
지문 8 1. How much we suffer relates to how we frame the pain in our mind.
2. When 1500m runners push themselves into extreme pain to win a race - their muscles screaming and their lungs exploding with oxygen deficit, they don't psychologically suffer much.
3. In fact, ultramarathon runners - those people who are crazy enough to push themselves beyond the normal boundaries of human endurance, covering distances of 50-100km or more over many hours, talk about making friends with their pain.
4. When a patient has paid for some form of passive back pain therapy and the practitioner pushes deeply into a painful part of a patient's back to mobilise it, the patient calls that good pain if he or she believes this type of deep pressure treatment will be of value, even though the practitioner is pushing right into the patient's sore tissues.
지문 9 1. When I worked for a large electronics company that manufactured laser and inkjet printers, I soon discovered why there are often three versions of many consumer goods.
2. If the manufacturer makes only one version of its product, people who bought it might have been willing to spend more money, so the company is losing some income.
3. If the company offers two versions, one with more features and more expensive than the other, people will compare the two models and still buy the less expensive one.
4. But if the company introduces a third model with even more features and more expensive than the other two, sales of the second model go up; many people like the features of the most expensive model, but not the price.
5. The middle item has more features than the least expensive one, and it is less expensive than the fanciest model.
6. They buy the middle item, unaware that they have been manipulated by the presence of the higher-priced item.
지문 10 1. Onscreen, climate disaster is everywhere you look, but the scope of the world's climate transformation may just as quickly eliminate the climate-fiction genre - indeed eliminate any effort to tell the story of warming, which could grow too large and too obvious even for Hollywood.
2. You can tell stories 'about' climate change while it still seems a marginal feature of human life.
3. But when the temperature rises by three or four more degrees, hardly anyone will be able to feel isolated from its impacts.
4. And so as climate change expands across the horizon, it may cease to be a story.
5. Why watch or read climate fiction about the world you can see plainly out your own window?
6. At the moment, stories illustrating global warming can still offer an escapist pleasure, even if that pleasure often comes in the form of horror.
7. But when we can no longer pretend that climate suffering is distant - in time or in place - we will stop pretending about it and start pretending within it.
지문 11 1. Today, the water crisis is political - which is to say, not inevitable or beyond our capacity to fix - and, therefore, functionally elective.
2. That is one reason it is nevertheless distressing: an abundant resource made scarce through governmental neglect and indifference, bad infrastructure and contamination, and careless urbanization.
3. There is no need for a water crisis, in other words, but we have one anyway, and aren't doing much to address it.
4. Some cities lose more water to leaks than they deliver to homes: even in the United States, leaks and theft account for an estimated loss of 16 percent of freshwater; in Brazil, the estimate is 40 percent.
5. Seen in both cases, as everywhere, the selective scarcity clearly highlights have-and-have-not inequities, leaving 2.1 billion people without safe drinking water and 4.5 billion without proper sanitation worldwide.
지문 12 1. As individuals, our ability to thrive depended on how well we navigated relationships in a group.
2. If the group valued us, we could count on support, resources, and probably a mate.
3. If it didn't, we might get none of these merits.
4. It was a matter of survival, physically and genetically.
5. Over millions of years, the pressure selected for people who are sensitive to and skilled at maximizing their standing.
6. The result was the development of a tendency to unconsciously monitor how other people in our community perceive us.
7. We process that information in the form of self-esteem and such related emotions as pride, shame, or insecurity.
8. These emotions compel us to do more of what makes our community value us and less of what doesn't.
9. And, crucially, they are meant to make that motivation feel like it is coming from within.
10. If we realized, on a conscious level, that we were responding to social pressure, our performance might come off as grudging or cynical, making it less persuasive.
지문 13 1. Conventional medicine has long believed that depression is caused by an imbalance of neurotransmitters in the brain.
2. However, there is a major problem with this explanation.
3. This is because the imbalance of substances in the brain is a consequence of depression, not its cause.
4. In other words, depression causes a decrease in brain substances such as serotonin and noradrenaline, not a decrease in brain substances causes depression.
5. In this revised cause-and-effect, the key is to reframe depression as a problem of consciousness.
6. Our consciousness is a more fundamental entity that goes beyond the functioning of the brain.
7. The brain is no more than an organ of consciousness.
8. If it is not consciousness itself, then the root cause of depression is also a distortion of our state of consciousness: a consciousness that has lost its sense of self and the meaning of life.
9. Such a disease of consciousness may manifest itself in the form of depression.
지문 14 1. The common accounts of human nature that float around in society are generally a mixture of assumptions, tales and sometimes plain silliness.
2. However, psychology is different.
3. It is the branch of science that is devoted to understanding people: how and why we act as we do; why we see things as we do; and how we interact with one another.
4. The key word here is 'science.'
5. Psychologists don't depend on opinions and hearsay, or the generally accepted views of society at the time, or even the considered opinions of deep thinkers.
6. Instead, they look for evidence, to make sure that psychological ideas are firmly based, and not just derived from generally held beliefs or assumptions.
7. In addition to this evidence-based approach, psychology deals with fundamental processes and principles that generate our rich cultural and social diversity, as well as those shared by all human beings.
8. These are what modern psychology is all about.

Copyright © 지인북스. All Rights Reserved.

사업자등록번호 415-92-01827 | 통신판매신고 2024-대전유성-1240 | 대표: 김유현
대전광역시 유성구 문화원로 13 | 고객센터: 010-4829-2520

이용 약관 개인정보 처리방침